The Seven Sins of Memory , by Daniel L. Schacter

Book Reviews : Science / Social Issues

Site main index :
Book Reviews - All Categories
 >> Science Books

History and Social Issues
 >> The Seven Sins of Memory
by Daniel L. Schacter

Please click here to check the price / option to buy :
The Seven Sins of Memory , by Daniel L. Schacter

Hardcover - 272 pages
First Edition, May 2001
Published by Houghton Mifflin Company
ISBN 0-6180-4019-6

            In an ancient tradition, the seven deadly sins were said to be pride, anger, envy, greed, lust, gluttony and sloth. Note that some are sins of commission (doing what we shouldn't) and others are sins of omission (failure to do what we should). Note also that each is an exaggeration of an essential need, for example, laziness is the extreme of our natural need for rest.

            In his new book, The Seven Sins of Memory , Harvard psychologist Daniel L. Schacter finds an analogy to the problems of imperfect human memory. Our memory commits the seven sins known as transience, absent-mindedness, blocking, misattribution, suggestibility, bias and persistence.

The Seven Sins of Memory by Daniel L. Schacter

            Each of memory's sins is a nuisance at times, but it is also necessary for survival and, for that reason, has developed in us through evolution by natural selection.

            Schacter is a leading authority on the process in which memory traces are recorded and retrieved by the brain, and how they can be lost due to brain damage. That research was the subject of his earlier book Searching for Memory , the winner of several science literature awards. In The Seven Sins of Memory , Schacter writes in plain English without overly technical jargon.

            Transience is the fading of memory with the passage of time. It's a nuisance when you can't remember the name of a person whom you met last year. Take comfort in the fact that we would be in real trouble if memory wasn't transient. Imagine how confused you would be if a telephone number you looked up a moment ago were exactly as prominent in your mind as every number you have ever seen if your life. Evolution gave our memories transcience because our survival depends on it. When you're about to eat, you want the immediate location of your food to be in the forefront of your thoughts, rather than obscured by equally vivid images of every place in the world where you have ever seen food.

Table of Contents

The Seven Sins of Memory ,
by Daniel L. Schacter


Acknowledgments                          ix

Introduction :
A Blessing Bestowed by the Gods           1

1. The Sin of Transience                 12
2. The Sin of Absent-mindedness          41
3. The Sin of Blocking                   61
4. The Sin of Misattribution             88
5. The Sin of Suggestibility            112
6. The Sin of Bias                      138
7. The Sin of Persistence               161
8. The Seven Sins : Vices or Virtues?   184

Notes                                   207
Bibliography                            230
Index                                   259

            Displaying absent-mindedness, a music student at UCLA drove off with the university's Stradivarius on the roof of his car. The institution recovered the instrument 27 years later. [43] Again, this fault is a gift from evolution because we couldn't function without it. Who could even take a step, or lift a finger, without the ability to put many details of bodily movements on automatic pilot, while we think about other things? [46]

            In each case, Schacter looks at the latest technology and what it reveals about the brain. For instance, blocking is the inability to retrieve specific information from memory, the tip-of-the-tongue experience. When this occurs, some interesting things show up if we happen to be looking at the frontal lobe of the brain with an MRI (magnetic resonance imaging) or a PET scanner (positron emission tomography). [47] He also considers the latest data on whether certain nutritional supplements can lessen such memory problems. [37]

            Some of the sins of memory ought to be serious public concerns. Misattribution is false memory. One believes that he or she remembers something which never actually occurred. Most cases are unimportant -- we may feel certain that a movie contains a line which doesn't really appear in the dialogue. Misattribution is a social problem largely because 75,000 criminal trials each year in the U.S. have outcomes that were determined by eyewitness testimony. [92]

            One type of misattribution is memory conjunction error, that is, imagining that two separate observations were together. [95] Experiments show that people who have seen a list of words containing "multiply" and "aptitude" will later answer in the affirmative when asked whether the list had contained the word "multitude." That seems harmless enough. But now consider the possibility that an innocent person may convicted of a crime, because an eyewitness who saw two people, one with a red beard and another with a large chin, later testified incorrectly in court to having seen the suspect, who has both a red beard and a large chin. It's exactly the same type of error as that which has occurred in the word list experiments, but now it isn't harmless.

            The chapter on suggestibility required the greatest effort on my part to read and absorb. An example illustrates what we mean by suggestibility :

            A psychological study was conducted ten months after the news carried the story of a plane crash. The crash itself was never televised. When the researchers asked subjects whether or not the crash was televised, most would recall correctly they had never seen such a broadcast. However, when the researchers asked subjects, "Did you see the television film of the moment when the plane hit the building?", most would reply in the affirmative. In the latter case, the wording of the question implied that the event had been televised. The subjects tended to combine this implication with a visualization of how a plane crash might look, and they thereby produced false memories of having seen the televised footage. [112]

            Now consider how suggestibility might lead to social problems. Police investigators asking crime witnesses to identify suspects have occasionally hinted to the witnesses that an individual is "already known to be the actual perpetrator" and "we only need you to confirm what we already know." If our memories are as susceptible to suggestion as the research indicates, this practice could be increasing the number of innocent suspects who get convicted of crimes. [115]

            In the chapter on suggestibility, I would have liked the author to spend a bit more time on the role of advertising, which he discusses briefly. Advertisers spend billions of dollars, not to inform the public about product characteristics, but to control our behavior, to implant in us the belief that we can never lead full and happy lives unless we switch to Brand X. We act subsequently as though we remember having learned that "fact" somewhere.

            At first, I began to misinterpret the chapter on bias. I failed to note that the term "bias" doesn't merely refer to biased interpretation of the present, such as prejudice. It must be understood in connection with inaccurate recollection of the past.

            Several types of bias are recognized. Consistency bias is is the error of overstating the extent to which the past and present are similar, and change bias is overestimating the extent to which the past and present are different. Both of those errors can be found in any political debate, as well as in any unhappy marriage. [139-144]

            Hindsight bias is the false feeling that "I knew it all along." [146]

            Egocentric bias is error resulting from exaggerating the role of the self, for example, the assumption that one's own memory must be more reliable than some else's. [150]

            Stereotypical bias involves forming generic memories, not of specific events, but of patterns or gists of experiences. There would have been no bigoted regime of Hitler without bias. However, without some stereotypical bias, we couldn't learn anything at all; indeed, we couldn't even think. We need to generalize the existence of such abstract categories as "tree" or "chair", as distinct from the particular instances of this tree or that chair. Therefore, we can't remove all stereotypes, but should remove those which are unwarranted by facts.

            In comprehending bias, imperfect recall of the past is the key to understanding distorted recognition of the present. Since we cannot tolerate cognitive dissonance, we may "... fabricate incidents that never happened in order to bring our memories in line with our expectations." [156] Schacter warns of the danger that someone may manipulate society by modifying records of the past, as in Orwell's novel 1984 . [138] The author also provides a three page discussion of the physical basis of bias in the structure of the brain. [157-160]

            Persistence is, to use Schacter's phrase, "when memory hurts." [165] It is the relatively clear memory of events that we would have preferred to see fade with time. Some people continue to be haunted years or decades after unpleasant experiences. Many combat soldiers have suffered from shell shock and post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD). [173] In 1986 a pitcher for the California Angels threw a ball that gave the Red Sox a home run which helped them win the World Series, then went into three years of severe depression ending with suicide. [161]

            Like the other sins of memory, persistence is an evolutionary adaptation. Our survival depended on it when we were hunter-getherers in the forest. People who have frightening memories of being attacked by bears are more likely to be careful to avoid future attacks by bears. Painful or not, evolution favors characteristics which increase the probability that people will survive to reproduce.

            These seven sins of memory, in this sequence, are the topics of the first seven chapters of the book. Chapter 8, entitled The Seven Sins : Vices or Virtues? revisits the issues raised in the first seven chapters, and then further clarifies the "seemingly contradictory relationship" [206] between the pleasant and unpleasant effects of our faulty memory.

Reviewed by Mike Lepore for
crimsonbird.com

            I recommend this book to readers interested in general psychology, social work and self-help practices, as well as the relationship between memory inaccuracies and such social problems as marital discord, racial and ethnic tensions, and erroneous verdicts in the criminal justice system.

            Hardcover only. 272 pages. 12-page 2-column index. 21 pages of notes as an appendix. 28 page bibliography. A few sketched illustrations; no photographs.

Please click here for price and shipping information ...
The Seven Sins of Memory , by Daniel L. Schacter

Book Description From the Publisher

The Seven Sins of Memory : How the Mind Forgets and Remembers
By Daniel Schacter
Published by Houghton Mifflin
May 2001; ISBN 0-6180-4019-6

A seminal book by one of the world's foremost memory experts
offers the first framework that explains common memory vices --
and their surprising virtues.

            Daniel L. Schacter, chairman of Harvard University's Psychology Department and a leading expert on memory, has developed the first framework that describes the basic memory miscues we all encounter. Just like the seven deadly sins, the seven memory sins appear routinely in everyday life. Schacter explains how transience reflects a weakening of memory over time, how absent-mindedness occurs when failures of attention sabotage memory, and how blocking happens when we can't retrieve a name we know well. Three other sins involve distorted memories: misattribution (assigning a memory to the wrong source), suggestibility (implanting false memories), and bias (rewriting the past based on present beliefs). The seventh sin, persistence, concerns intrusive recollections that we cannot forget -- even when we wish we could. Although these sins may cause difficulties, as Schacter notes, they're surprisingly vital to a keen mind.

            Schacter, whose previous trade book, Searching for Memory , was called "splendidly lucid" (The New Yorker), offers vivid examples of the memory sins -- for instance, the absent-mindedness that plagued both a national memory champion and a violinist who forgot that he had placed a priceless Stradivarius on top of his car before driving off. The author also delves into the recent research -- such as imaging that shows memories being formed in the brain -- which has led him to develop his framework. Together, the stories and the scientific findings examined in The Seven Sins of Memory provide a fascinating new look at our brains, and at what we more generally think of as our minds.

            The Seven Sins of Memory is a groundbreaking work that will provide great reassurance to everyone, from twenty-somethings who find their lives are too busy, to baby boomers who mutter about "early Alzheimer's," to senior citizens who worry about how much (or how little) they can recall.

About the Author

            Daniel L. Schacter is chairman of the Psychology Department at Harvard University. He has previously written Searching for Memory , which received praise as a New York Times Notable Book and a Library Journal Best Science and Technology Book of the Year, and won the American Psychological Association's William James Book Award. He lives in Newton, Massachusetts.

Reviews

            "To a very great extent, our memories are our selves. The Seven Sins of Memory is a gripping and thought-provoking exploration of this eternally fascinating topic. Written by one of the world's experts, it presents startling examples from the news and everyday life and explains them using an original and elegant theory. The ideas are fascinating, and of great importance both for self-knowledge and for intelligent public policy."

-- Steven Pinker,
professor of psychology at MIT and author of
Words and Rules : The Ingredients of Language
and
How the Mind Works

            "Bravo: a tour de force. No one can better explain for the general reader the new insights on memory and its distortion than Daniel Schacter, one of the most exciting and original students of the biology of memory."

-- Eric R. Kandal, M.D.,,
University Professor at Columbia University,
senior investigator at the Howard Hughes Medical Institute,
and 2000 Nobel laureate

            "Want to know what memory is, how it works, and how to work around its failures? Read this book! Seven Sins commits none."

-- Joseph LeDoux,
Henry and Lucy Moses Professor of Science,
Center for Neural Science, New York University,
and author of
The Emotional Brain : The Mysterious
Underpinnings of Emotional Life

            "Schacter bestows on us a rare gift -- a fascinating journey through paths of memory, its open avenues and blind alleys. With sparkling prose and lively narratives, he challenges us to reconceive apparent defects of memory as crucial assets in successfully sustaining a sharp and productive mind. Rarely have I received such a lucid, engaging, and enjoyable book about science."

-- Jerome Groopman, M.D.,
professor of medicine at Harvard Medical School,
staff writer at The New Yorker,
and author of
The Measure of Our Days : A Spiritual Exploration of Illness
and
Second Opinions : Stories of Intuition and Choice
in a Changing World of Medicine

Check the price ...   Amazon.com link for
ISBN 0-6180-4019-6 , The Seven Sins of Memory , by Daniel L. Schacter (Nonfiction)
Site main index :
Book Reviews - All Categories
 >> Science Books

History and Social Issues
 >> The Seven Sins of Memory
by Daniel L. Schacter